Composition Matters aims to describe a phenomenon which is overlooked by many. Visual harmony is typically equated with compositional balance, achieved through not very precisely-defined proportions. However, there is also a state of perfect harmony accomplished by some artworks, whereby introducing any perceptible change means reducing the aesthetic value of the work as a whole.
This is the proposed harmony algorithm. It demonstrates the relationship that needs to occur between all elements in a composition in order to create the aforementioned perfect state. Yet it does not intend to make any evaluative judgements about art.
Composition Matters attempts to address the following questions:
What is the mechanism of visual harmony?
How does visual harmony affect perceptions of beauty?
What is visual sensitivity?
***
Put simply, the composition of a painting is all that remains once we disregard the style (or painterly conventions), the content, and the associated cultural and emotional context. Composition is the purely visual interplay between the elements which make up a painting. It is very difficult for an artist to create an excellent composition; it is similarly challenging for the viewer to recognise it as such. Managing the interplay between elements to achieve a state of perfect visual harmony requires the extraordinary ability to perceive all compositional relationships on the canvas. Throughout history, very few artists have had the skill to succeed in this quest, and even fewer viewers have appreciated this skill.
What follows does not aim to popularise knowledge of appropriate compositional construction. Instead, it targets those who, engaged in creating or evaluating paintings of any type, have already noticed that composition matters and would like to explore the mechanisms behind it.
***
Much has been written on visual perception. Yet so far, nobody has considered analysing harmony as a state where the introduction of any change would perceptibly reduce the aesthetic value of the work as a whole.
Visual harmony has been investigated before. Rudolf Arnheim, a renowned art and film theorist and perceptual psychologist, explored how various relationships between compositional elements affect viewers’ emotions. He analysed a range of visual phenomena, including the division of a surface, balance, weight, perception of elements as parts of a whole, spatial perception, and deformation.
Max Wertheimer, together with the psychologists Wolfgang Köhler and Kurt Koffka, developed Gestalt theory based on the phenomenon of holistic perception – the mind’s tendency to instinctively combine elements into commonly-known or expected forms. However, its similarity to the matter of perfect harmony is only illusory, stemming from its use of the terms “part” and “whole”.
In the case of Composition Matters, the “whole” is simply the entire painting – a complex structure composed of many smaller parts, not a commonly-known or expected simple form. Naturally, there are artists who use deformations and simplifications in representing commonly-known forms, thus employing what Gestalt theory has taught us about perception. However, this does not mean that they create what is here considered a harmonious composition.
The renowned art historian Ernst Gombrich writes on the subject of pictorial representation:
What an artist worries about as he plans his picture, makes his sketches, or wonders whether he has completed his canvas, is something much more difficult to put into words. Perhaps he would say he worries about whether he has got it ‘right’. Now it is only when we understand what he means by that modest little word ‘right’ that we begin to understand what artists are really after. […] When it is a matter of matching forms or arranging colours an artist must always be ‘fussy’ or rather fastidious to the extreme. Moreover, his task is infinitely more complex than any of those we may experience in ordinary life. He has not only to balance two or three colours, shapes or tastes, but to juggle with any number. He has, on his canvas, perhaps hundreds of shades and forms he must balance till they look ‘right’. A path of green may suddenly look too yellow because it was brought into close proximity with a strong blue – he may feel that all is spoiled, that there is a jarring note in the picture and that he must begin it all over again. He may suffer agonies over this problem. He may ponder about it on sleepless nights; he may stand in front of his picture all day trying to add a touch of colour here or there and rubbing it out again, though you and I might not have noticed the difference either way. But once he has succeeded, we all feel that he has achieved something to which nothing could be added, something which is right – an example of perfection in our very imperfect world. [*]
* E. H. Gombrich, The Story of Art, Phaidon Press, New York, 1951, p. 14